Why do psychedelics
I think she may well be right. Simply spending this much time observing my mind and having experiences where I got to sneak up on it in various ways does have an effect. We often think about science and spirituality as these opposed terms, but in fact a lot of this research is forcing scientists to deal with spiritual questions, and some spiritual people to deal with scientific questions, which is very exciting.
The very first study in the modern era of psychedelic research, of any importance, was a study done at Johns Hopkins by a scientist named Roland Griffiths, a very prominent drug-abuse scientist. It has various aspects to it. Prominent among them is this dissolving of a sense of self, but that is followed by a merging with the universe, or with nature, or other people.
We see this experience all over religious literature: people who have had an experience of meeting with the divine.
These traits are common, and the fact that you could induce such a spiritual experience with a single administration of a drug was quite remarkable. These people reported that this experience was one of the top two or three in their lives, comparable to the birth of a child or the death of a parent.
Now that we can actually induce a spiritual experience using a drug, we can study the phenomenon. It made me more curious and a little less afraid. On this psilocybin trip, I saw the faces of people close to me who had died over the last few years. You understand why traditional cultures would take plant medicines to reconnect with the dead. You can see them and talk to them and they can talk to you. I wrote this book during a period when my dad was dying.
He had terminal cancer, and I dedicated this book to him before he died. One of the things the psilocybin research is doing is helping open that conversation — to make people more comfortable talking about it, to get patients to actually deal with it.
So a drug that takes you into these spiritual realms where you can begin to think it through seems to me an enormous gift. I had the experiences I needed to have. I met people who have a psychedelic experience once a year on their birthday, and that seemed about right, to do that sort of stock-taking. Write to Mandy Oaklander at mandy. Reports indicate that there are a number of different versions of NBOMe available — all with differing effects. Effects of psychedelics There is no safe level of drug use.
Psychedelics affect everyone differently, based on: size, weight and health whether the person is used to taking it whether other drugs are taken around the same time the amount taken the strength of the drug varies from batch to batch.
Using psychedelics with other drugs The effects of mixing psychedelics with other drugs, including alcohol, prescription medications and over-the-counter medicines, are often unpredictable.
Health and safety There is no safe way to use psychedelic drugs. It is difficult to predict the strength and effects of psychedelics even if they have been taken before , as the strength and potency can vary from batch to batch.
People with mental health conditions or a family history of these conditions should avoid using psychedelics. Taking psychedelics in a familiar environment in the company of people who are known and trusted may alleviate any unpleasant emotional effects. Anxiety can be counteracted by taking deep, regular breaths while sitting down.
Dependence and tolerance Most psychedelics produce tolerance rapidly and psychological dependence can occur in some people. Path2Help Not sure what you are looking for? Find out more. Psychonaut Wiki.
Psychedelics Psychonaut Wiki. DOM Dance Safe. Controlled psychophysical studies have measured various alterations in motion perception Carter et al. In what are known as elementary hallucinations —e. In complex hallucinations visual scenes can present elaborate structural motifs, landscapes, cities, galaxies, plants, animals, and human and non-human beings Shanon, ; Studerus et al. Complex hallucinations typically succeed elementary hallucinations and are more likely at higher doses Kometer and Vollenweider, ; Liechti et al.
CEVs are often described as vivid mental imagery. Under psychedelic drugs, mental imagery becomes augmented and intensified—e. Psychedelic mental imagery can be modulated by both verbal Carhart-Harris et al.
Somatosensory perception can be drastically altered—e. Sense of time and causal sequence can lose their usual linear cause-effect structure making it difficult to track the transitions between moments Heimann, ; Wittmann et al. Overall the perceptual effects of psychedelics are extremely varied, multimodal, and easily modulated by external stimuli.
Perceptual effects are tightly linked with emotional and cognitive effects. Emotional psychedelic effects are characterized by a general intensification of feelings, increased conscious access to emotions, and a broadening in the overall range of emotions felt over the duration of the drug session. Psychedelics can induce unique states of euphoria characterized by involuntary grinning, uncontrollable laughter, silliness, giddiness, playfulness, and exuberance Preller and Vollenweider, Negatively experience emotions—e.
However, the majority of emotional psychedelic effects in supportive contexts are experienced as positive Studerus et al. Both LSD and psilocybin can bias emotion toward positive responses to social and environmental stimuli Kometer et al. Spontaneous feelings of awe, wonder, bliss, joy, fun, excitement and yes, peace and love are also consistent themes across experimental and anecdotal reports Huxley, ; Kaelen et al.
In supportive environments, classic psychedelic drugs can promote feelings of trust, empathy, bonding, closeness, tenderness, forgiveness, acceptance, and connectedness Dolder et al.
Emotional effects can be modulated by all types of external stimuli, especially music Bonny and Pahnke, ; Shanon, ; Kaelen et al. Precise characterization of cognitive psychedelic effects has proven enigmatic and paradoxical Shanon, ; Carhart-Harris et al. Acute changes in the normal flow of linear thinking—e. This is reflected in reduced performance on standardized measures of working memory and directed attention Carter et al.
Crucially, cognitive impairments related to acute psychedelic effects are dose-dependent Wittmann et al. Theoretical attempts to account for the reported effects of microdosing have yet to emerge in the literature and therefore present an important opportunity to future theoretical endeavors.
Certain cognitive traits associated with creativity can increase under psychedelics Sessa, ; Baggott, such as divergent thinking Kuypers et al. Furthermore, long-term increases in creative problem-solving ability Sweat et al. Ego dissolution is more likely to occur at higher doses Griffiths et al. Furthermore, certain psychedelic drugs cause ego dissolution experience more reliably than others; psilocybin, for example, was found to produce full ego dissolution more reliably compared with LSD Liechti et al.
Ego dissolution experiences can be driven and modulated by external stimuli, most notably music Carhart-Harris et al. Mescaline-assisted therapies showed promising results during first-wave psychedelic science Beringer, b ; Rouhier, and this trend continued through second-wave psychedelic research on LSD-assisted therapies Sandison and Whitelaw, ; Cohen and Eisner, ; Pahnke et al.
Recent studies have produced significant evidence for the therapeutic utility of psychedelic drugs in treating a wide range of mental health issues Tupper et al. In many clinical studies, ego-dissolution experience has correlated with positive clinical outcomes Griffiths et al.
Remarkably, as mentioned above, a single psychedelic experience can increase optimism for at least 2 weeks after the session Carhart-Harris et al.
A study of regular weekly ayahuasca users showed improved cognitive functioning and increased positive personality traits compared with matched controls Bouso et al. Interestingly, these outcomes may expand beyond sanctioned clinical use, as illicit users of classic psychedelic drugs within the general population self-report positive long-term benefits from their psychedelic experiences Carhart-Harris and Nutt, , are statistically less likely to evidence psychological distress and suicidality Hendricks et al.
The above evidence demonstrates the broad diversity of acute subjective effects that classic psychedelic drugs can produce in perceptual, emotional, and cognitive domains. Unique changes in sense of self, ego, body image, and personal meaning are particularly salient themes. How do these molecules produce such dramatic effects? What are the relationships between acute perceptual, emotional, cognitive, and self-related effects?
What is the link between acute effects and long-term changes in mental health, personality, and behavior? Theories addressing these questions emerged as soon as Western science recognized the need for a scientific understanding of psychedelic drug effects beginning in the late 19th century.
The effects described above are what captured the interest of first-wave and second-wave psychedelic scientists, and the theories they developed in their investigations have two central themes. The first theme is the observation that psychedelic effects share descriptive elements with symptoms of psychoses, such as hallucination, altered self-reference, and perceptual distortions.
The second theme is the observation that psychedelic drugs seem to expand the total range of contents presented subjectively in our perceptual, emotional, cognitive, and self-referential experience. A third theoretical account uses psychoanalytic theory to address the expanded range of mental phenomena produced by psychedelic drugs as well as the shared descriptive elements with symptoms of psychoses. The next section reviews these themes along with their historically associated theories before tracing their evolution into third-wave 21st-century psychedelic science.
Model psychoses theory began long before any of the classic psychedelic drugs became known to Western science. The dramatic subjective effects of mescaline invigorated the model psychoses paradigm. Growing demand for the ideal chemical agent for model psychoses eventually motivated Sandoz Pharmaceuticals to bring LSD to market in the s. Nonetheless, it seeded the idea that psychedelic effects themselves could be explained in terms of psychopathology and motivated a search for common neural correlates.
The founding figures of neuropharmacology were driven by questions regarding the relationship between psychoactive drugs and endogenous neurochemicals see Abramson, The discovery that LSD can antagonize serotonin led to the hypothesis that the effects of LSD are serotonergic and simultaneously to the historic hypothesis 6 that serotonin might play a role in regulating mental function Gaddum, ; Gaddum and Hameed, ; Woolley and Shaw, ; Shaw and Woolley, ; Green, At the Second Conference on Neuropharmacology the whole class of drugs was dubbed psychotomimetic Abramson, Psychoto mimetic drug effects, on this literal reading of the term, would merely mimic or imitate—appear as if they are—psychoses.
However, to mimic is not to model. A mimic, by contrast, merely creates the illusion that it possesses the properties it mimics. Thus, the term psychotomimetic implies that the effects of these drugs merely resemble psychoses but do not share functional or structural properties in their underlying biology or phenomenology. Nonetheless, LSD and mescaline were used as models to investigate psychotic symptoms.
A subtle explanation-explananda circularity can come into play here, in which psychoses are explained using drug models yet the drug effects are explained using theories of psychoses. Further complicating the matter is the clear difference between acutely induced drug effects and the gradual development of a chronic mental illness Osmond and Smythies, An additional conceptual challenge was the fact that mescaline had for years shown promise in treating psychopathologies Beringer, b ; Rouhier, and LSD was gaining popularity for pharmaceutically enhanced psychotherapy Sandison and Whitelaw, ; Eisner and Cohen, ; Cohen and Eisner, Model psychoses theory needed to explain how it was the case that drugs putatively capable of inducing psychotic symptoms could simultaneously be capable of treating them—What Osmond , p.
Taken together, the above cluster of conceptual challenges drove Osmond to doubt his own prior work on model psychoses Hoffer et al. It looks as though the most satisfactory working hypothesis about the human mind must follow, to some extent, the Bergsonian model, in which the brain with its associated normal self, acts as a utilitarian device for limiting, and making selections from, the enormous possible world of consciousness, and for canalizing experience into biologically profitable channels.
Yours sincerely ,. Aldous Huxley. Huxley b , p. Filtration theorists include founding figures of psychopharmacology Kraepelin, , psychology James, , and parapsychology Myers, , along with early 20th-century philosophers Bergson , and Broad Smythies , p.
In this new descriptive model, psyche mind delic manifesting drugs manifest the mind by inhibiting certain brain processes which normally maintain their own inhibitory constraints on our perceptions, emotions, thoughts, and sense of self. Osmond and Huxley both found this principle highly applicable to their own direct first-person knowledge of what it is like to experience the effects of mescaline and LSD—the expanded range of feelings, intensification of perceptual stimuli, vivid vision-like mental imagery, unusual thoughts, and expanding or dissolving sense of self and identity.
First, the pharmacological disruption of hypothetical inhibitory brain mechanisms that normally attenuate internal and external stimuli suggested that the kinds of effects produced by the drug would depend on the kinds of stimuli in the system, which is consistent with the diverse range of effects on multiple perceptual modalities, emotional experience, and cognition.
This picture helped Huxley and Osmond understand the relationship between psychedelic phenomena and psychotic phenomena: temporarily opening the cerebral reducing valve with psychedelics could produce mental phenomena that resembled symptoms of chronic natural psychoses precisely because both were the result of acute or chronic reductions in brain filtration mechanisms. Thus, filtration theory offered a way to understand psychedelic effects that was consistent with both their psychotomimetic properties and their therapeutic utility.
Osmond and Huxley argued that filtration theory concepts were fully consistent with the subjective phenomenology, psychotomimetic capability, and therapeutic efficacy of psychedelic drugs. However, it remains unclear exactly what it is that the brain is filtering and consequently what it is that emerges when the filter is pharmacologically perturbed by a psychedelic drug.
So which is it? Do psychedelic drugs manifest latent aspects of mind or of world? How we answer this question will crucially determine our ontological and epistemological conclusions regarding the nature of psychedelic experience.
Huxley and Osmond did not make this clear. Huxley seems to favor the position that psychedelic experience reveals a wider ontological reality and grants epistemic access to greater truth. Still, if mind provides us with access to world, then lifting restrictions on mind could in principle expand our access to world. Freud developed an elaborate theoretical account of mental phenomena which, like filtration theory, placed great emphasis on inhibition mechanisms in the nervous system.
Nonetheless, psychedelic drugs produce dream like visions and modes of cognition that feature symbolic imagery, conceptual paradox, and other hallmark characteristics of the primary process Carhart-Harris and Friston, ; Kraehenmann et al. How did other psychoanalytic theorists describe psychedelic drug effects? The core idea is that psychedelic drugs interfere with the structural integrity of the ego and thereby reduce its ability to suppress the primary process and support the secondary process Grof, Due in part to the close resemblance between psychedelic effects and primary process phenomena, psychoanalytic theory became the framework of choice during the mid 20th-century boom in psychedelic therapy Sandison, ; Sandison and Whitelaw, ; Cohen, ; Grof, ; Merkur, Psychedelic ego effects, which range from a subtle loosening to a complete dissolution of ego boundaries, were found to be great tools in psychotherapy because of their capacity to perturb ego and allow primary process phenomena to emerge Sandison, , p.
But how do psychedelic drugs disrupt the structure of the ego? Freud hypothesized that the organizational structure of ego rests upon a basic perceptual schematic of the body and its surrounding environment. Continuous correct perception is necessary to maintain ego feeling and ego boundaries.
Such barriers would presumably consist of processes limiting the spread of excitation between different functional areas of the brain.
The indications are that LSD, in some manner, breaks down these stimulus barriers of which Freud spoke. Nor is this merely a figure of speech. There is some reason to suspect that integrative mechanisms within the central nervous system CNS which handle inflowing stimuli are no longer able to limit the spread of excitation in the usual ways.
We might speculate that LSD allows greater energy exchanges between certain systems than normally occurs, without necessarily raising the general level of excitation of all cortical and subcortical structures. Klee, , p. Psychedelic drugs, according to Savage and Klee, perturb integrative mechanisms that normally bind and shape endogenous and exogenous excitation into the structure of the ego. From the above analysis of first-wave and second-wave theories I have identified four recurring theoretical features which could potentially serve as unifying principles.
A second feature is the hypothesis that this core brain mechanism can behave pathologically, either in the direction of too much, or too little, constraint imposed on perception, emotion, cognition, and sense of self. A third feature is the hypothesis that psychedelic phenomena and symptoms of chronic psychoses share descriptive elements because they both involve situations of relatively unconstrained mental processes.
A fourth feature is the hypothesis that psychedelic drugs have therapeutic utility via their ability to temporarily inhibit these inhibitory brain mechanisms. But how are these inhibitory mechanisms realized in the brain? Klee recognized that his above hypotheses, inspired by psychoanalytic theory and LSD effects, required neurophysiological evidence. What clues have recent investigations uncovered? A psychedelic drug molecule impacts a neuron by binding to and altering the conformation of receptors on the surface of the neuron Nichols, The receptor interaction most implicated in producing classic psychedelic drug effects is agonist or partial agonist activity at serotonin 5-HT receptor type 2A 5-HT 2A Nichols, Importantly, while the above evidence makes it clear that 5-HT 2A activation is a necessary if not sufficient mediator of the hallmark subjective effects of classic psychedelic drugs, this does not entail that 5-HT 2A activation is the sole neurochemical cause of all subjective effects.
Moreover, most psychedelic drug molecules activate other receptors in addition to 5-HT 2A e. How does psychedelic drug-induced 5-HT 2A receptor agonism change the behavior of the host neuron?
Generally, 5-HT 2A activation has a depolarizing effect on the neuron, making it more excitable more likely to fire Andrade, ; Nichols, Importantly, this does not necessarily entail that 5-HT 2A activation will have an overall excitatory effect throughout the brain, particularly if the excitation occurs in inhibitory neurons Andrade, In what ways do temporal oscillations change under psychedelic drugs?
MEG and EEG studies consistently show reductions in oscillatory power across a broad frequency range under ayahuasca Riba et al. Reductions in the power of alpha-band oscillations, localized mainly to parietal and occipital cortex, have been correlated with intensity of subjective visual effects—e. Under LSD, reductions in alpha power still correlated with intensity of subjective visual effects but associated alpha reductions were more widely distributed throughout the brain Carhart-Harris et al.
Furthermore, ego-dissolution effects and mystical-type experiences e. The concept of functional connectivity rests upon fMRI brain imaging observations that reveal temporal correlations of activity occurring in spatially remote regions of the brain which form highly structured patterns brain networks Buckner et al. Imaging of brains during perceptual or cognitive task performance reveals patterns of functional connectivity known as functional networks ; e. In what ways does brain network connectivity change under psychedelic drugs?
Third, brain networks that normally show anticorrelation become active simultaneously under psychedelic drugs.
This situation, which can be described as increased between-network functional connectivity, occurs under psilocybin Carhart-Harris et al. Fourth and finally, the overall repertoire of explored functional connectivity motifs is substantially expanded and its informational dynamics become more diverse and entropic compared with normal waking states Tagliazucchi et al.
Notably, the magnitude of occurrence of the above four neurodynamical themes correlates with subjective intensity of psychedelic effects during the drug session. Furthermore, visual cortex is activated during eyes-closed psychedelic visual imagery de Araujo et al. Taken together, the recently discovered neurophysiological correlates of subjective psychedelic effects present an important puzzle for 21st-century neuroscience. A key clue is that 5-HT 2A receptor agonism leads to desynchronization of oscillatory activity, disintegration of intrinsic integrity in the DMN and related brain networks, and an overall brain dynamic characterized by increased between-network global functional connectivity, expanded signal diversity, and a larger repertoire of structured neurophysiological activation patterns.
Crucially, these characteristic traits of psychedelic brain activity have been correlated with the phenomenological dynamics and intensity of subjective psychedelic effects. How should we understand the growing body of clues emerging from investigations into the neurodynamics of psychedelic effects? What are the principles that link these thematic patterns of psychedelic brain activity or inactivity to their associated phenomenological effects?
Recent theoretical efforts to understand psychedelic drug effects have taken advantage of existing frameworks from cognitive neuroscience designed to track the key neurodynamic principles of human perception, emotion, cognition, and consciousness.
The overall picture that emerges from these efforts shares core principles with filtration and psychoanalytic accounts of the late 19th and early 20th century. Briefly, normal waking perception and cognition are hypothesized to rest upon brain mechanisms which serve to suppress entropy and uncertainty by placing various constraints on perceptual and cognitive systems. The core hypothesis of recent cognitive neuroscience theories of psychedelic effects is that these drugs interfere with the integrity of neurobiological information-processing constraint mechanisms.
The net effect of this is that the range of possibilities in perception, emotion, and cognition is dose-dependently expanded. From this core hypothesis, cognitive neuroscience frameworks are utilized to describe and operationalize the quantitative neurodynamics of key psychedelic phenomena; namely, the diversity of effects across many mental processes, the elements in common with symptoms of psychoses, and the way in which temporarily removing neurobiological constraints is therapeutically beneficial.
This section is organized according to the broad theoretical frameworks informing recent theoretical neuroscience of psychedelic effects: entropic brain theory, integrated information theory , and predictive processing.
Their hypothesis states that hallmark psychedelic effects e. Importantly, EBT points out that these characteristics are consistent with psychedelic phenomenology, e. Entropic Brain Theory further characterizes psychedelic neurodynamics using a neo-psychoanalytic framework proposed in an earlier paper by Carhart-Harris and Friston , p.
Importantly, this hypothesis maps onto the subjective phenomenology of psychedelic effects, particularly ego dissolution. This view, based on Freudian metapsychology, is also consistent with filtration accounts, like those of Bergson and Huxley, who hypothesized that psychedelic drug effects are the result of a pharmacological inhibition of inhibitory brain mechanisms.
In normal waking states, the DMN constrains the activity of its cortical and subcortical nodes and prohibits simultaneous co-activation with TPNs. By interfering with DMN integration, psychedelics permit a larger repertoire of brain activity, a wider variety of explored functional connectivity motifs, co-activation of normally mutually exclusive brain networks, increased levels of between-network functional connectivity, and an overall more diverse set of neural interactions.
Carhart-Harris et al. However, what may be gained in mild depression i. Thus, EBT formulates all four of the theoretical features identified in filtration and psychoanalytic accounts, but does so using 21st-century empirical data plugged into the quantitative concepts of entropy, uncertainty, criticality, and functional connectivity.
EBT hints at possible ways to close the gaps in understanding by offering quantitative concepts that link phenomenology to brain activity and pathogenesis to therapeutic mechanisms. Integrated Information Theory IIT is a general theoretical framework which describes the relationship between consciousness and its physical substrates Oizumi et al.
Gallimore formally restates this problem using IIT parameters: brains attempt to optimize the give-and-take dynamic between cause-effect information and cognitive flexibility. In IIT, a neural system generates cause-effect information when the mechanisms which make up its current state constrain the set of states which could casually precede or follow the current state. In other words, each mechanistic state of the brain: 1 limits the set of past states which could have causally given rise to it, and 2 limits the set of future states which can causally follow from it.
Thus, each current state of the mechanisms within a neural system or subsystem has an associated cause-effect repertoire which specifies a certain amount of cause-effect information as a function of how stringently it constrains the unconstrained state repertoire of all possible system states. Increasing the entropy within a cause-effect repertoire will in effect constrain the system less stringently as the causal possibilities are expanded in both temporal directions as the system moves closer to its unconstrained repertoire of all possible states.
Moreover, increasing the entropy within a cause-effect repertoire equivalently increases the uncertainty associated with its past and future causal interactions.
Using this IIT-based framework, Gallimore argues that, compared with normal waking states, psychedelic brain states exhibit higher entropy, higher cognitive flexibility, but lower cause-effect information Figure 4. Neuroimaging data suggests that human brains exhibit a larger overall repertoire of neurophysiological states under psychedelic drugs, exploring a greater diversity of states in a more random fashion. The cause-effect repertoire of brain mechanisms thus shifts closer to the unconstrained repertoire of all possible past and future states.
Therefore the subjective contents perception and cognition become more diverse, more unusual, and less predictable. This increases flexibility but decreases precision and control as the subjective boundaries which normally demarcate distinct cognitive concepts and perceptual objects dissolve.
Gallimore leverages IIT in an attempt unify these phenomena under a formalized framework. How do psychedelic drugs increase neural entropy? The first modern brain imaging measurements in humans under psilocybin yielded somewhat unexpected results: reductions in oscillatory power MEG and cerebral blood flow fMRI correlated with the intensity of subjective psychedelic effects Carhart-Harris et al.
In their discussion, the authors suggest that their findings, although surprising through the lens of commonly held beliefs about how brain activity maps to subjective phenomenology, may actually be consistent with a theory of brain function known as the free energy principle FEP; Friston, In one model of global brain function based on the free-energy principle Friston, , activity in deep-layer projection neurons encodes top-down inferences about the world. Speculatively, if deep-layer pyramidal cells were to become hyperexcitable during the psychedelic state, information processing would be biased in the direction of inference—such that implicit models of the world become spontaneously manifest—intruding into consciousness without prior invitation from sensory data.
This could explain many of the subjective effects of psychedelics Muthukumaraswamy et al. What is FEP? FEP is a formulation of a broader conceptual framework emerging in cognitive neuroscience known as predictive processing PP; Clark, PP has links to bayesian brain hypothesis Knill and Pouget, , predictive coding Rao and Ballard, , and earlier theories of perception and cognition MacKay, ; Neisser, ; Gregory, dating back to Helmholtz who was inspired by Kant ; see Swanson, At the turn of the 21st century, the ideas of Helmholtz catalyzed innovations in machine learning Dayan et al.
PP subsumes key elements from these efforts see Clark, to describe a universal principle of brain function captured by the idea of prediction error minimization PEM; Hohwy, What does it mean to say that the brain works to minimize its own prediction error?
Higher-level areas of the nervous system i. Crucially, the sets of possible causes must be narrowed in order for the system to settle on an explanation Tenenbaum et al. For example, the brute constraints of space and time act as hyperpriors; e. Interestingly, PP holds that our perceptions of external objects recruit the same synaptic pathways that enable our capacity for mental imagery, dreaming, and hallucination.
How do psychedelic molecules perturb predictive processing? The idea is that psychedelic drugs perturb the learned and innate prior constraints on internal generative models.
How does PP tie into filtration theories and psychoanalytic accounts? One objection to this linkage might be that Huxley often describes psychedelic opening of the cerebral reducing valve as revealing more of the world.
At first glance this seems at odds with the above PP account of psychedelic effects, which describes psychedelic drugs causing rampant internal simulations of reality, not revealing more of the external world.
However, this apparent tension might be resolved in light of active inference , a key principle of FEP Friston, Active inference shows how internal models do not merely generate top-down inference signals but also shape the sampling and accumulation of bottom-up sensory evidence signals. This is known as active inference. Psychedelics manifest mind by perturbing prior constraints on internal generative models, thereby expanding the possibilities in our inner world of feelings, thoughts, and mental imagery.
The brief speculative remark by Muthukumaraswamy et al. The PP framework describes a recurrent back-and-forth give-and-take between colliding top-down and bottom-up signals, where internal models serve to shape experience and experience serves to build internal models, so this leaves room for rival PP-based accounts that diverge regarding where exactly the psychedelic drug perturbs the system.
For example, increased top-down activity could be the result of pharmacological hyperactivation of top-down synaptic transmission; yet equally plausible is the hypothesis that increased top-down activity is a compensatory response to pharmacological attenuations or distortions of bottom-up signal.
For example, Corlett et al. The issue of what is primary and what is compensatory illustrates the vast possibilities in the hypothesis space of PP-based accounts. While most PP-based accounts point to changes in top-down signaling, even within this hypothesis space there are contrasting conceptions of exactly how psychedelic molecules perturb top-down processing.
Briefly, these differing hypotheses include: 1 hyperactivation or heavier weighting of top-down signaling Muthukumaraswamy et al. Carhart-Harris and Friston argue that the Freudian conception of ego, with its organizing influence over the primary process, is consistent with PP descriptions of higher-level cortical structures predicting and suppressing the excitation in lower levels in the hierarchy i.
In normal adult waking states, networks based in higher-level areas can successfully predict and explain suppress and control the activity of lower level areas. PP posits that the brain explains self-generated stimuli by attributing its causes to a coherent and persisting entity i.
Letheby and Gerrans , p. Pink-Hashkes et al. Categorical predictions that are more detailed, by contrast, will carry less precision and thus potentially generate more prediction error Kwisthout and van Rooij, ; Kwisthout et al.
In summary, the current state of PP-based theories of psychedelic effects reveals a divergent mix of heterogeneous ideas and conflicting hypotheses.
Apply market research to generate audience insights. Measure content performance. Develop and improve products. List of Partners vendors. Hallucinogens are a class of drugs that can cause hallucinations or sensations and images that seem real even though they are not. For centuries, hallucinogens also known as psychedelics have been used by people in many cultures for religious rituals, by artists to spark creativity, or for recreation.
The reasons people try hallucinogens are varied, but for most, they alter perception, thoughts, and feelings. Though most are not addictive, some may be, and there may be some risks and benefits involved with hallucinogenic use. Hallucinogenic and dissociative drugs are used for a variety of reasons. Hallucinogenic and dissociative drugs are used for social and recreational use.
Some may take hallucinogenic drugs simply to escape life's troubles or to relieve boredom. Hallucinogens are sometimes used in spiritual pursuits to produce mystical "visions" or simply to induce a detachment from reality in order to be closer to mythical beings.
Historically, hallucinogens were used in shamanic practices of indigenous cultures and some are even incorporated in religions such as that of the Native American Church. Writers, poets, and artists have used hallucinogens and other drugs through the decades to find creative inspiration.
People who have mental or emotional issues might try hallucinogens simply to alter their state of mind. In fact, hallucinogens have been investigated as a way to aid in the psychotherapeutic process for some. Although not approved for such use at this time, some hallucinogenic drugs have been scientifically tested to see if they might have therapeutic effects in mood, substance use, and anxiety disorders. According to research published in , anecdotal reports and small studies have suggested that ayahuasca may be a potential treatment for substance use disorders and other mental health issues, but no large-scale research has verified its efficacy.
Research suggests that hallucinogens work, at least partially, by temporarily disrupting communication between chemical systems throughout the brain and spinal cord. Some hallucinogens interfere with the action of the brain chemical serotonin. Serotonin can affect mood, sensory perception, sleep, hunger, body temperature, sexual behavior, and muscle control.
0コメント